Vocal mimicry on the web

« previous post | next post »

We haven't had anything recently about how clever starlings are, but what with all the discussion about parrot lips, I thought that some of you might enjoy this:

There are no associated news stories, so far, about vocal organs or communication skills, though commenters on several web forums have made suggestions about demonic possession and (from those who listen more carefully) the possible dangers of keeping vocal mimics as house pets.

Some background on starling vocalizations:

Some interesting references (with links and abstracts) about the (still very puzzling) phenomenon of vocal mimicry:

Laura Kelley, Rebecca Coe, Joah Madden, and Susan Healy, "Vocal mimicry in songbirds", Animal Behavior 76(3):521-528, 2008.

Baylis (1982, Acoustic Communication in Birds, Academic Press) decried the serious lack of experimental verification for the various hypotheses proposed to explain vocal mimicry in songbirds. With few exceptions, our understanding of the function and acquisition of this fascinating behaviour seems to have scarcely progressed. We examine the proposed functional explanations and supporting evidence, and summarize advances made since Baylis's (1982) review. We conclude that there is no compelling evidence to support any of the functional hypotheses but, rather, that almost all of the data concerning song mimicry are consistent with the learning mistakes hypothesis, whereby birds learn simple and common sounds, frequently using them in inappropriate contexts. Additionally, many apparently mimicked sounds are calls, not songs, which themselves may not be learned by the models. It is plausible that many examples of call mimicry are, in fact, due to evolutionary convergence.

Martha Leah Chaiken and Jörg Böhner, "Song learning after isolation in the open-ended learner the European Starling: Dissociation of imitation and syntactic development", The Condor 109(4) 2007.

We performed two studies to test whether the ability of open-ended learners to acquire new songs as adults depends on their having learned normal songs as juveniles. European Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) were kept in isolation for their first year. In the first study the birds were housed in a group with a wild-caught adult male following isolation. The subjects imitated each other but not the wild male and failed to develop normal phonology or syntax. In the second study each yearling was housed individually with a wild-caught adult male following isolation. These subjects developed good phonology and syntax but copied few or no song motifs from the wild adults. Taken together, the two studies indicate that starlings are capable of imitating new motifs and of acquiring species-typical phonology and syntax after a year of isolation. The contrasting results of the two studies suggest that imitation and the development of syntax are independent processes subject to different influences.

László Zsolt Garamszegi et al., "A comparative study of the function of heterospecific vocal mimicry in European passerines", Behavioral Ecology 18(6):1001-1009, 2007.

Although heterospecific vocal imitation is well documented in passerines, the evolutionary correlates of this phenomenon are poorly known. Here, we studied interspecific variation in vocal mimicry in a comparative study of 241 European songbirds. We tested whether vocal mimicry is a mode of repertoire acquisition or whether it resulted from imperfect song learning. We also investigated the effect of the degree of contact with the vocal environment (with species having larger ranges, abundance, or being long lived having a higher degree of mimicry) and a possible link with cognitive capacity (an overall larger brain in species with mimicry). Finally, we determined the potential evolutionary role of vocal mimicry in different interspecific contexts, predicting that mimicry may affect the intensity of brood parasitism, predation, or degree of hybridization. While controlling for research effort and phylogenetic relationships among taxa, we found that effect sizes for intersong interval, brain size, breeding dispersal, abundance, age-dependent expression of repertoires, and predation risk reached a level that may indicate evolutionary importance. Vocal mimicry seems to be a consequence of song continuity rather than song complexity, may partially have some cognitive component but may also be dependent on the vocal environment, and may attract the attention of predators. However, estimates of sexual selection and interspecific contacts due to brood parasitism and hybridization varied independently of vocal mimicry. Therefore, mimicry may have no function in female choice for complex songs and may be weakly selected via interspecific associations. These findings provide little evidence for vocal mimicry having evolved to serve important functions in most birds.

Eben Goodale and Sarath W Kotagama, "Context-dependent vocal mimicry in a passerine bird", Proc Biol Sci. 273(1588):875–880, 2006.

How do birds select the sounds they mimic, and in what contexts do they use vocal mimicry? Some birds show a preference for mimicking other species' alarm notes, especially in situations when they appear to be alarmed. Yet no study has demonstrated that birds change the call types they mimic with changing contexts. We found that greater racket-tailed drongos (Dicrurus paradiseus) in the rainforest of Sri Lanka mimic the calls of predators and the alarm-associated calls of other species more often than would be expected from the frequency of these sounds in the acoustic environment. Drongos include this alarm-associated mimicry in their own alarm vocalizations, while incorporating other species' songs and contact calls in their own songs. Drongos show an additional level of context specificity by mimicking other species' ground predator-specific call types when mobbing. We suggest that drongos learn other species' calls and their contexts while interacting with these species in mixed flocks. The drongos' behaviour demonstrates that alarm-associated calls can have learned components, and that birds can learn the appropriate usage of calls that encode different types of information.

Kazuo Okanoya, "Language evolution and an emergent property", Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17(2):271-276, 2007.

Much debate has been stimulated by the recent hypothesis that human language consists of a faculty that is shared with non-human animals (faculty of language in a broad sense; FLB) and a faculty that is specific to human language (faculty of language in a narrow sense; FLN). This hypothesis has encouraged a tendency to emphasize one component of FLN: the cognitive operation of recursion. In consequence, non-syntactical, yet unique, aspects of human language have been neglected. One of these properties consists of vocal learning that enables an abundance of learned syllables. I suggest that FLN is not an independent faculty, but an ‘emergent’ property, arising from interactions between several other non-syntactical subfaculties of FLB, including vocal learning ability.



9 Comments

  1. misterarthur said,

    November 1, 2008 @ 9:45 am

    Interesting post. My father-in-law, Stewart Hulse, (who just passed away) was one of the originators of the field of Animan Cognition, and used Starlings in his research. I remember him telling me that startlings could not only differentiate between Beethoven and Bach, but between Bach and Buxtehude. (One of Bach's contemporaries.

  2. Eyebrows McGee said,

    November 1, 2008 @ 9:58 am

    We have a huge starling flock that migrates through (and torments my cats), and a couple years back, one of them was mimicking a very popular cell phone ring. I thought I was going to absolutely kill myself by the time he left!

    Another one discovered that if he mimicked the fire engine sirens (I live near a firehouse), it would start the nearest dog barking, who would set off every dog in the neighborhood. Then he would wait for the barking to stop … and do the siren again. This apparently provided hours of entertainment for the bird … and DAYS AND DAYS of nearly non-stop barking outbursts.

    My favorite mimicked the sounds of children playing on a playground as heard from two blocks away. (Distant shouting, periodic shrieks and laughter, distant ball sounds, etc.) It was uncanny.

  3. alwen said,

    November 1, 2008 @ 10:05 am

    I live out in a rural area, and it is very disconcerting when starlings start doing cell phone rings up in the trees. I'm used to being alone and not hearing those!

  4. sumrandom said,

    November 1, 2008 @ 11:42 am

    The Australian Lyre Bird does a similar thing (we don't get starlings). I once visited one in a wildlife sanctuary that was using a variety of other bird's calls as well as the sound of a circular saw to try and impress a female. Apparently it had picked it up from a nearby DIY-er.

  5. Sili said,

    November 1, 2008 @ 2:58 pm

    I'm obviously no good at paraphonia(?) either. I couldn't make much sense of anything it said.

    As the talking cat shows, subtitles are a must …

    I do recall seing film by Attenborough with a jungle bird exceptionally good at mimicry. It did a pretty good motorsaw and for its pièce de resistance mimicked the sound of a camera shutter.

  6. Kevin Iga said,

    November 1, 2008 @ 3:37 pm

    The Attenborough clip of a lyrebird is here.

  7. Bobbie said,

    November 1, 2008 @ 3:55 pm

    It's eerie to start wondering exactly what that bird is talking about. Sounds quite intriguing, but not intelliglble. Does it sound "sexy" to anyone else?

    I was aware that mockingbirds.parrots, and parakeets (budgies) have the capability to mimic other birds (and music) but did not know that starliings can do this too.

  8. dr pepper said,

    November 1, 2008 @ 11:14 pm

    This is so much more interesting than those stupid one dimensional "talking critter" reports.

  9. Joe said,

    November 6, 2008 @ 10:23 am

    Has anyone verified the origin of European Starlings in North America? Apparently, in the 1890s, the American Acclimatization Society decided it was a good idea to import every bird that Shakespeare mentioned. The specific line in "Henry V" ("The king forbade my tongue to speak of Mortimer. But I will find him when he is asleep, and in his ear I'll holler 'Mortimer!' Nay I'll have a starling shall be taught to speak nothing but Mortimer, and give it to him to keep his anger still in motion.") describes the bird as an annoyance. As interesting starlings are, some folks regard them as a major nuisance as well. If you've ever raised a newborn in an area populated by these birds and their sophisticated cacophony, you may perhaps wish that someone in the Society had read Shakespeare a little better.

RSS feed for comments on this post